Menu Sign In Contact FAQ
Banner
Welcome to our forums

Disorientation Simulation

https://www.amst.co.at/en/aerospace-medicine/training-simulation-products/airfox/airfox-diso/

I’ve just had the pleasure of being put through the mill in this machine for disorientation training. Very revealing day about how our vestibular system can mess us up, particularly with instrument flight / visual illusions and incipient instrument failures. The machine can induce some quite nasty effects of the leans but it was great to experience them and how to mitigate. Thought provoking stuff – I certainly came away thinking that simulator instrument training could perhaps do with some more of these elements, my IR(R) students could really benefit from a bit more scenario based failure I am thinking…

Now retired from forums best wishes

Thank you for sharing. I was interested in attending a test of (probably) this sim but it was too far away to cover expenses.

always learning
LO__, Austria

Yes, I think the range of 3-axis accelerations and rotations plus instruments marks that machine can put you trough may go beyond what you will experience in aircraft using flight controls, so you will surely get some nasty experiences: while that does not make them real they will emphasis a heavy reliance on instruments or even the side effect of questioning them?

Pushing the yoke, while AI pitch up and a working ASI stay static and feeling acceleration and turn is not a realistic scenario but has enough to blow your mind ;)

I got my worse experiences of spatial desorientation 1/ real flying in clear day with a nasty cold (I know one should not but it was hard to say no to the instructor) and 2/ flying with VR headsets on a rotating chair with few instrument failures (seems from low resolution of gauges than actual lean effects)

Last Edited by Ibra at 01 Mar 19:20
Paris/Essex, France/UK, United Kingdom

@Balliol do you think this training helps with instrument flying, or is its main value in weeding out pilots who perhaps cannot learn instrument flying because they are too heavily affected by the illusions?

I wonder why that facility was set up. It must have been very expensive.

Administrator
Shoreham EGKA, United Kingdom

@peter I think the intent is so that pilots can experience the effects and learn how to recognise and mitigate against them in a safe environment. We also went through looking at ‘black hole effect’ and some visual illusions with runways on cliff tops, descending through a low, shallow cloud layer, runway slopes etc.. In the military environment (which is where this setup is and we have to go through it every 5 years) disorientation can be massively dangerous in the formation situation, and particularly around the tanker in Air-to-Air Refuelling.

Now retired from forums best wishes

Ibra wrote:

Yes, I think the range of 3-axis accelerations and rotations plus instruments marks that machine can put you trough may go beyond what you will experience in aircraft using flight controls

This is probably correct for the aircrafts we usually fly but in fighter jets (and I understood this is a military facility) you can easily get these forces and nasty disorientation caused by confusing inputs.

LDZA LDVA, Croatia

Emir wrote:

you can easily get these forces and nasty disorientation caused by confusing inputs.

A dog fight in IMC or in the Alps will be fun

Paris/Essex, France/UK, United Kingdom

I wonder who has the job of cleaning up after each session!

EIWT Weston, Ireland
… or is its main value in weeding out pilots who perhaps cannot learn instrument flying because they are too heavily affected by the illusions?

That proposition is suspiciously similar to the view, espoused by Major Isaac Jones of the US Army in 1917, which was abandoned by 1922 following research by Dr Wulfften Palthe among others (see following pdf). If we are to believe Alan J Benson, former senior medical officer at RAF institute and School of Aviation Medicine, the low prevalence of vestibular asymmetry among pilots would make that a wasteful objective. Benson in his Spatial Disorientation – A Perspective, pp KN-3 — KN-4, (pdf link) local copy writes:

In the 60s and 70s I was involved in carrying out tests of vestibular function on all aircrew who
were referred to the RAF neuropsychiatrist because of S.D. This work did show that, as a group, there was
slightly more, and marginally significant, vestibular asymmetry in those aircrew with S.D. than in a
control group. In some cases the demonstration of an asymmetry in yaw or roll axis sensation cupulograms
explained the illusion experienced by the pilot (Benson,1973a).

Benson, while later acknowledging the difficulty in validating the effectiveness of ground-based SD exposure, states in the context of instrument flight training (p KN-7):

Most now accept that flight experience should be complemented by specific instruction about S.D.
as well as by a demonstration in which the student aviator experiences some of the perceptual errors that
can be engendered by the unfamiliar motion and visual stimuli of the flight environment. Such a
demonstration of the fallibility of human perception is most convincingly achieved in actual flight, and
effective protocols have been developed for in-flight demonstration of S.D.. However, considerations of
cost and flight safety have led to the much greater use of ground-based S.D. demonstrators.

Dr David G Newman, in an ATSB research and analysis report, series B2007/0063 — An overview of spatial disorientation as a factor in aviation accidents and incidents (link), states:

[Spatial disorientation] is
a very common problem, and it has been estimated that the chance of a pilot
experiencing SD during their career is in the order of 90 to 100 per cent. The results
of several international studies show that SD accounts for some six to 32 per cent of
major accidents, and some 15 per cent to 26 per cent of fatal accidents. The true
prevalence of SD events is almost certainly underestimated.

John Richard Rollin Stott in a Jan 2013 article in Extreme Physiology & Medicine, Orientation and disorientation in aviation (link), citing a survey of UK military accidents over the periods 1983–1992 and 1993–2002, notes:

Of particular significance in this survey were the
findings that 50% of disorientation-related accidents
involved distraction and that, at the point at which
the accident became inevitable, disorientation remained
unrecognised in 85% of accidents.

That is a staggering preponderance involving type I (unrecognised) spatial disorientation so it’s little wonder the author concludes (p 9) “that a disorientated pilot does not experience an illusion.” Referring to a spatial disorientation survey of experienced instructor pilots conducted by Sipes Lessard in 2000 (see footnote 12), Stott notes the authors “found that, of those incidents rated by the reporting pilot as significant or severe in relation to flight safety, 75% involved unrecognised disorientation.”

That survey was done 1997–1998 at the US Air Force Advanced Instrument School, Randolph AB, and involved 141 participating airmen. About 90 per cent of them were active-duty military pilots and all but one of the remainder, a navigator, were retired military pilots working as simulator contractors. Flying experience ranged from 500 to 10 000 hours with a mean of 2886 hours and standard deviation of 1271 hours (Sipes and Lessard, pp 35–36). Results are reproduced below. The table summarises the number and percentage of airmen reporting experience of a particular illusion. The last three columns record the means, standard deviations, and ranges of occurrences. An additional table below, based on the survey, briefly describes the illusions.

Table 2. Descriptive Statistics of Spatial Disorientation Illusions Reported by 141 Surveyed Aircrew
Name of Disorientation

No. of Pilots
Reporting the Illusion

% Mean SD Range
Leans 132 94 12 21 1-200
Black hole 111 79 12 21 1-112
Blending of the sky and the earth 89 63 12 24 1-100
Loss of situational awareness 81 57 13 35 1-200
Autokinesis 76 54 7 16 1-100
Coriolis illusion 73 52 7 14 1-100
Sloping clouds or terrain 73 52 4 4 1-20
Brownout 64 45 9 22 1-100
Misperceived self-moving on formation 61 43 5 5 1-50
False cues in formation flight 61 43 5 5 1-15
Instrument confusion 56 40 5 5 1-20
Instrument reversal 53 38 4 3 1-10
Giant hand 49 35 4 4 1-20
Whiteout 42 30 8 21 1-100
Flicker vertigo 39 28 5 6 1-30
False pitch up from acceleration 38 27 7 18 1-100
Rotating beacon illusions 23 16 4 4 1-15
Strobe light reflections 38 27 5 4 1-12
G-excess illusion 32 23 6 11 1-50
Dip illusion 31 22 8 18 1-100
False pitch down with deceleration 31 22 7 18 1-100
Elevator illusion 30 21 7 7 1-30
Instrument failure 29 21 2 2 1-10
False cues from the Northern Lights 25 18 4 5 1-20
Aerial flares 22 16 6 9 1-40
Illusions using night vision goggles 20 14 8 15 1-50
Graveyard spiral 19 14 2 2 1-10
Inversion illusion 16 11 3 3 1-10
Nystagmus 15 11 4 2 1-6
Lean on the sun 14 10 3 3 1-5
Illusions (forward looking infrared radar) 12 9 4 3 1-10
False vertical from the Moon 11 8 3 2 1-5
Break-off phenomenon 8 6 17 34 2-100
Illusions using head-up display 8 6 10 17 2-50
Moth effect 7 5 2 1 1-5
Illusions using helmet-mounted devices 3 2 2 1 1-2
Illusions using a targeting pod 3 2 3 1 2-4
Graveyard spin 1 .7 1 1 1

Illusion Name Illusion Description
Leans a false banking sensation usually in a turn
Black Hole Approach an approach to a dark area short of the runway
Blending of Earth and Sky trouble keeping orientation due to poor or no distinct horizon
Loss of Situational Awareness disorientation due to loss of perception of overall picture
Autokinesis the apparent motion of a single light
Coriolis Illusion sensation of rolling and/or pitching after abrupt head movements
Sloping Clouds or Terrain falsely flying by orienting on a sloping cloud or terrain horizon
Brownout lack of a distinct horizon due to blowing sand or dust
Misperceived Self Motion in Formation a false sensation of movement when other aircraft move
No Vertical or Horizontal Cues in Formation disorientation due to lack of visual cues in formation
Flight instrument confusion disorientation due to inability to quickly interpret the flight instruments
Flight instrument reversal banking opposite the intended direction
Giant Hand Illusion false banking sensation of a giant hand pushing down on one of the wings
Whiteout disorientation with a lack of horizon due to atmospheric whiteout or blowing snow
Flicker Vertigo disorientation due to pulsing light (rotor blades, propellers, lights flashing)
Takeoff/Acceleration Pitch Up a false sensation of pitching up on takeoff or accelerating in flight
Rotating Illusion a false yaw sensation caused by the anticollision light reflecting off cloud or fog
Strobe Light false sensations caused by strobe reflections
G-excess Illusion false or exaggerated sensation of body tilt (e.g. penetration or procedure turn)
Dip Illusion in night trail formation, the trail A/C flies lower than expected due to poor visual cues
Rapid Deceleration Pitch Down a false nose-down pitch sensation with abrupt deceleration (speed brakes)
Elevator Illusion false sensation of upward or downward motion as in an elevator
Flight instrument failure disorientation due to instrument failure and the unresolved conflict
Northern Lights false cues of the vertical based on the northern lights
Aerial Flares false cues of vertical caused by drifting flares
Night Vision Goggle Involved Illusion disorientation due to use of night vision goggles
Graveyard Spiral loss sensation in a banked turn, upon perceived roll out, the pilot reenters the turn
Inversion Illusion a false sensation of being upside down, usually after an abrupt level-off
Nystagmus loss of visual acuity on instruments as the eyes compensate for vestibular stimulation
Lean on the Sun in clouds the pilot uses the diffused sunlight as up and banks towards the sun
FLIR involved illusions disorientation due to perceptions with Forward Looking Infra-Red Radar
Moon Illusion (false vertical) – falsely using the moon for vertical orientation
Break-Off feeling of detachment from the earth at high altitude or in poor visibility
Head Up Display disorientation caused by a conflict with the use of a HUD
Moth Effect a flight path of a descending spiral to keep orientation around a descending flare
Helmet Mounted Device disorientation caused by a conflict with the use of a HMD
Targeting Pod disorientation caused by the use of a targeting pod
Graveyard Spin loss of the sensation of a spin and upon perceived recovery, the pilot reenters the spin

London, United Kingdom

Very interesting, thank you!

always learning
LO__, Austria
13 Posts
Sign in to add your message

Back to Top